Wednesday, 16 May 2012

Is exposure to angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors related to Alzheimer’s disease?


(first published on Nature Network May 11th)  


One of the questions I have been investigating is whether a type of anti-hypertensive drug, angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors, may inadvertently cause some people to develop Alzheimer’s disease.

There is currently no cure for Alzheimer’s disease.  Patients diagnosed with dementia on average survive for 4 years.  Alzheimer’s disease was first described in 1906, but scientists do not know exactly what causes the biological changes characteristic of the disease.  We do know Alzheimer’s disease gets more common as people get older:


Data from MRC CFAS study.1

This hypothesis around angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors was motivated by research suggesting that angiotensin converting enzymes may have a role in clearing amyloid-β plaques from the brain.  So angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors which, as their name suggests, inhibit angiotensin converting enzyme, may increase the build up of amyloid-β, increasing the likelihood of Alzheimer’s disease.2

The ideal way to answer this question would be to do an experiment, which randomly allocates some people to angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors and others to another anti-antihypertensive, and then see whether there were differences in the number of diagnosed with Alzheimer’s.  However, the effects of angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors are only thought to build up after long periods of exposure.  So participants would need to be followed in the experiment for a very long time.  Mercifully, Alzheimer’s disease is relatively rare in younger people, so we would either need to run the experiment in older people, or in a very large number of people to detect any difference in diagnosis rates.  This could be hugely expensive.

Some epidemiologists, Anderson and colleagues, did the next best thing.3  They followed-up an experiment that had already been run, the ONTARGET study.  In which the outcomes of people given angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors were compared to those given another anti-hypertensive drug, angiotensin receptor blockers. 

They investigated whether the participants experienced differences in cognitive impairments, defined as a diagnosis of dementia, or had a low score in a cognitive test, the mini-mental state examination depending on the treatment they were allocated.  They found that people allocated to angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors were around 10% more likely to develop cognitive impairments over the four years of the experiment,(odds-ratio 1.11 95%CI (0.99,1.25) p-value =0.06).  However, they found little difference in another outcome, cognitive decline (a fall in the mini-mental state exam score).

These findings are certainly not conclusive.  This might be because the participants of the ONTARGET experiment were relatively young, average age of 66, so few participants would be expected to develop Alzheimer’s disease.  Also the ONTARGET trial’s primary outcome was death from cardiovascular causes, myocardial infarction, stroke, or hospitalization for heart failure.  This means it did not necessarily have enough data to detect an effect of the drugs on cognitive impairments or cognitive decline.

Another way to investigate whether angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors cause Alzheimer’s disease, is to compare the outcomes of patients prescribed ACE-Is and other anti-hypertensive drugs as part of their normal medical care.  Some more epidemiologists, Li and colleagues, did this using data from the United States.4

They found that patients prescribed lisinopril (an angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitor) were 23% more likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease than those prescribed angiotensin receptor antagonists, (odds-ratio 1.23 (95%CI:1.04,1.47), p<0.001).  Patients prescribed lisinopril were slightly more likely to develop Alzheimer’s disease than patients given other cardiovascular drugs (such as statins) odds-ratio 1.04 (95%CI: 0.99,1.11, p=0.15).  But this association was relatively small weak and could be due to chance.  Whist Li and colleagues adjusted their findings for their patients’ characteristics; it is possible that these differences are due to underlying differences between patients prescribed lisinopril and angiotensin receptor antagonists in their sample.  For instance patients prescribed angiotensin receptor antagonists might be richer or younger.

I looked into this using data from the General Practice Research Database.5  This is contains administrative data on diagnoses and prescriptions from over 600 general practices in the UK.  We found evidence that fewer patient prescribed angiotensin converting enzymes were diagnosed with Alzheimer’s than those prescribed other anti-hypertensives.  However, when we looked at historical exposure to angiotensin converting enzymes we found little evidence of an association. Again this is frustratingly inconclusive.

The only way to conclusively prove whether long term exposure to angiotensin converting enzyme inhibitors is related to Alzheimer’s disease is with a randomised controlled trial of sufficient size in older people with higher risks for the disease.

Oh also Louis Theroux’s most recent show on dementia is really interesting, catch it on iplayer.

And here’s podcast from Professor June Andrews on care services for people with dementia.

I'd hear your thoughts on this and our other posts, and do pass on any papers or links.
 
1. Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study(MRC CFAS). Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicentre, community-based population in England and Wales. Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). Lancet. 2001;357(9251):169–175.
2. Kehoe P, Miners S, Love S. Angiotensins in Alzheimer’s disease-friend or foe? Trends Neurosci. 2009;32(12):619–628.
3. Anderson C, Teo K, Gao P, et al. Renin-angiotensin system blockade and cognitive function in patients at high risk of cardiovascular disease: analysis of data from the ONTARGET and TRANSCEND studies. The Lancet Neurology. 2011;10(1):43–53.
4. Li N, Lee A, Whitmer R, et al. Use of angiotensin receptor blockers and risk of dementia in a predominantly male population: prospective cohort analysis. BMJ. 2010;340(jan12 1):b5465.
5. Davies NM, Kehoe PG, Ben-Shlomo Y, Martin RM. Associations of anti-hypertensive treatments with Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and other dementias. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2011;26(4):699–708.

Wednesday, 9 May 2012

Mendelian Randomisation and the Prevention of Spurious Findings

(first published on Nature Network April 19th)

Epidemiology uses statistical methods to investigate patterns in public health. Or, if you believe certain news articles, it splits every possible life influence into things that either cause or cure cancer (or occasionally do both). Epidemiologists investigate health outcomes in a number of different ways, ranging from looking at changes in whole populations to conducting randomised controlled trials (RCTs), testing one intervention against another, or against the placebo effect. RCTs are the 'Gold Standard' of Epidemiological research, as their design means they can provide the strongest evidence for or against a hypothesis. Other epidemiological methods involve observation, rather than manipulation, and this can be problematic.

One week there may be a paper suggesting vitamin pills protect against heart disease, and the next week evidence emerges that they don't. This is often because of misleading findings from observational studies. People who decide to take vitamin supplements are likely to be different in a number of ways from people who don't take them. They might lead a more healthy lifestyle, exercise more, smoke less, and eat more vegetables. These differences are likely to impact on their chance of getting heart disease. Epidemiologists call these differences confounding variables, and although it's possible to take them into account in various statistical ways, the scientist has to know what they are in order to control for them. Miss one confounder out of your analysis, and it may inflate any relationship between the two factors you're interested in, so you've got a spurious finding. One way of getting round this problem is to conduct RCTs, so different types of people are randomly distributed across your intervention condition. Although this may be fine to do with a vitamin pill, it's sometimes not ethical or even possible. If withholding the intervention being investigated is thought to lead to harm, it is unethical to do so, and if the intervention is something like drinking alcohol, it's impractical to get people to either drink or not drink for an experiment.

Investigating alcohol in observational studies has the same problems mentioned above; people who drink are different to people who don't. Socioeconomic status, likelihood to smoke and education level may all affect the relationship you're interested in. There are added complications in that people may have stopped drinking because they are unwell, so instead of drinking affecting the disease, the disease is changing alcohol use. This is called reverse causation, and may explain why we hear that a glass of wine is good for you (sorry to be the bringer of bad news).

But advances in the understanding of genetics can help. Everyone has 23 pairs of chromosomes, and coded in these are genes; the building blocks for who we are. My genes are practically identical to yours, but there are some key genes which have different DNA code in different people. Some of these differences can lead to diseases, such as Cystic Fibrosis, caused by a faulty gene not properly making an important protein. Some have less extreme, but still very interesting, effects. There has been a gene variation found, common in East Asian populations, which means a protein needed to break down a metabolite of alcohol is not produced. People with this variation get unpleasant symptoms when they drink due to a build up of acetaldehyde in their blood, so very often they avoid alcohol. By looking at someone's DNA in the location known here, we can investigate the effect of alcohol use on whatever we're interested in, using the gene as a proxy variable instead of directly analysing alcohol intake, as people with the unusual variation will be less likely to drink. All well and good, but surely this is still just observing, so how do we stop the interference of confounding?

It turns out that our genes have some very useful properties which make them perfect for this task. The genes that you have are all unrelated to environment, as you got them before you were born, so environmental confounders should be randomly distributed between your gene categories. Also, when your parents' chromosomes divided to create the egg or sperm that contained the genes you inherited from them, each gene splits independently of all the others, so you have a random chance of also inheriting a genetic confounder. Because of these neat properties, you can assume that your proxy gene will be independent of any confounding variable affecting the exposure you're interested in, and therefore where confounder levels would have been uneven across people grouped by alcohol consumption, they will be randomly distributed across gene variation. Results using this technique have shown evidence against alcohol being a gateway drug leading to illicit drug use, as the gene variation was not associated with illicit drug use.

Of course, nothing's perfect. There are a few conditions where this technique will fall down, but as long as you're aware of them, you should be able to avoid the problems. Firstly, occasionally 'linkage disequilibrium' occurs. Certain genes are more likely to move together during meiosis, meaning they are not inherited independently. If your proxy gene travels with a gene which affects your outcome of interest, this will impact on your findings. There is a method to check for linkage disequilibrium, so you can ensure it's not a problem. Also, there are certain genes which have an impact on a number of different traits (this is called pleiotropy), so if your gene has a direct effect on the outcome you are interested in, it is unsuitable for Mendelian Randomisation. Finally, the technique fails to work effectively if there are systematic differences in the genetics of the population you are investigating. For example, if a population is made up of two groups of peoples that used to live separately, but now live together, there will be non random genetic differences between the groups due to selective mating over the time when they were separate. This may mean other differences between the groups will not be randomly distributed across the gene you're interested in, making the population unsuitable.

However, although this technique can only be used in very specific circumstances, where a gene is known to affect the intervention you're interested in, and doesn't suffer from the limitations mentioned above, it is a really elegant technique which will hopefully stop the spurious results from observational studies becoming newspaper fodder.


Check this article out for an overview of MR.

Tuesday, 8 May 2012

Ding Ding All Change

If you're wondering why it's been quiet here recently, change is afoot!

A couple of weeks ago this blog moved to Nature Network. Very exciting times for us! We'll still be updating this page with the posts that appear on our blog there (which is still called Sifting the Evidence, and can be found here), but they will appear a couple of days after. Feel free to look at them and enjoy them on either site!

Tuesday, 14 February 2012

Geek Like Me?

Should scientists steer clear of identifying themselves as geeks in case it puts off ‘more rounded’ individuals from taking up a STEM (science technology engineering and mathematics) career?

Monday, 5 December 2011

Who are the Children of the 90s?

If you were down on Bristol Harbourside this weekend you may have noticed an event at MShed, or heard mention of ‘Children of the 90s’. Who are these children, and what were they doing at the museum? The answer involves a journey more than 20 years back in time, and the vision and foresight of Bristol Professor Jean Golding.

Wednesday, 9 November 2011

It’s demography stupid!

The protestors on the streets of London, New York, Madrid and Tel Aviv do not understand the economic and political problems they face.  The developed world is suffering from a demographic crisis.  Railing against financiers and the richest 1% misses the underlying cause of the crisis: the costs of paying for their own parents' and grandparents' retirements.

Tuesday, 1 November 2011

First Class Mutations

Beasts on a plane!

It almost feels like a fool’s errand to isolate something that can help us make light of ourselves from the litany of improbable happenings taking place in X-Men: First Class. Any film with a sapphire coloured beast piloting a stealth fighter jet will struggle to poignantly frame questions about the evolution of humanity. But if you squint a little, the band of ‘mutant’ heroes, whose variety of supernatural traits are powered by a single mutation in the fictitious ‘X gene’, actually epitomise a core mechanism central to the field of population genetics.